Thinking Fast And Slow by Daniel Kahneman – Book Summary
In his book, Thinking Fast And Slow, Kahneman talks about human psychology, particularly the way we think and make decisions. He discusses two ways of thinking, one which is fast and intuitive, and the other which is slow and rigorous. He argues that the quick method is not always accurate, and it is sometimes better to slow down and use a thorough process of thinking to make better decisions. The book not only explains how we think but also gives us a primer on how to think better.
The
book is aimed towards more serious readers, who are looking for a rigorous
read. While no previous knowledge is necessary to read and comprehend the text,
readers have to apply effort to follow the narrative.
This
book is great for all those who want to know more about human psychology and
the thinking process. And to understand how the human mind works in thinking
fast and slow.
1- The Two Sets Of Thinking System In Our Mind
The two
systems of our brain process information in different ways to arrive at
different outcomes. The first system is intuitive. This system is an
evolutionary legacy of the past, wherein quick responses often conferred
survival advantages. The intuition which we are so often proud of is a trait of
this system of decision making. The instinctive or intuitive reactions are
somewhat automatic. Ducking to a ball thrown at your face is one such example
of system one. Intuitive and natural responses are reactionary and quick.
The
second system is that of deliberate and conscious decision-making. While
deciding which career to choose, one will go over the options carefully, and
weigh the pros and cons before making a choice. It is this
kind of system which resonates with our sense of self and conscious
individual decision-making.
Thus,
we are blessed with both ways i.e. thinking fast and slow.
Due to
this dichotomy, however, we are prone to making errors in judgment.
2- Out Of Thinking Fast And Slow, Which Path Our Mind Follows To Get To Results?
Our
intuitive thinking is the mind’s way of using the least energy to solve a
problem or direct an appropriate response. The natural system is such that
whenever system one is inept at explaining or handling a particular situation,
the more purposeful system two springs into action.
However,
this call to action is sometimes missed by our mind being fooled into assessing
a position in a different manner than it really is. That’s why our brain
develops the two modes of thinking i.e. fast and slow.
Kahneman
discusses a famous bat and ball problem, to showcase the errors in our
approach. Imagine you have a baseball and a bat. Both cost 1.1 dollars in
total, and the bat costs one dollar more than the ball. How much does the ball
cost? If you took less than a second to answer that question, the chances are
that you think that the ball cost 0.1 dollars!
It is
obvious from the above example that sometimes our brains are fooled into
adopting a system one approach towards solving a problem when ideally system
two should have been called. The real answer, in this case, is 0.05 dollars,
which is evident if you think about it for a bit and do some mental
mathematics.
The
problem presented above exposes our mind’s tendency to adopt the least effort
route to solve a problem, resulting in misjudgments sometimes.
3- We Are Not Conscious Agent Of Our Thoughts And Decisions
Priming
is a phenomenon that is utilized as a technique in psychology, wherein an
external stimulus influences an organism’s response to a subsequent stimulus
unconsciously.
Research
shows that people recognize certain words far quicker if they are primed with a
semantically similar word previously. For example, your brain will register the
word Nurse much faster, when it follows the word Doctor (semantically related)
than it would if it follows the word Day (semantically unrelated).
The
process of priming works at an unconscious level, and only sophisticated tests
can show its evidence. Priming is not just verbal; it can also be visual. As
such we are being primed with all sorts of visual and verbal cues left to right
and center all day, during our interaction with the world.
It is a
powerful phenomenon that can even affect our actions and decisions. In a
research, it was shown that a set of people who were primed with words related
to the elderly, walked out of the tests much more slowly than those who were
not primed with such words! So much for the belief that we are always in
control of our behavior and responses.
4- Role Of Forecasting In Making Faster Decisions
Forecasting
helps our minds to make snap judgments. Situations that are an ideal case for
heuristic judgments are those which need to be solved quickly to achieve
immediate goals. The approach is not designed to be optimal, but fast. However,
when presented with certain situations, sometimes our mind tends to over-rely
on these systems. We tend to jump ahead of the facts. Here is where System two
can play an essential role by causing us to doubt our own conclusions.
Often
when faced with a message, we are more likely to focus on the content of the
message than to doubt its relevance. This is because System one focuses on the
content, while System two sows the seeds of doubt. Since employing System two
takes effort, we are more likely to just believe in the data than to doubt it.
We also tend to answer questions that seem easier than the ones which have
really been asked. This is often the case when the problem presented is a
challenging one. Our mind with the overarching goal of solving it substitutes
the actual problem with an illusory one to provide an answer! This is evident
in the case of stereotypes. In a one-to-one conversation with a stranger, you
may judge his or her intelligence, not on objective calculation (which is
complex), but on the racial stereotype, you have in your own mind (easy
substitution).
Then
there is the availability bias, wherein our expectation of an event (such as a
natural disaster) is biased because one may have heard a lot about it, or it
may have happened in the recent past. For instance, immediately after a
hurricane or an earthquake, people seem to take more preparation for such
events, e.g., buying property insurance. Though they are more likely to die in
a road accident, their assessment of the threat that hurricanes pose to them
would be far graver. As time passes by, the level of preparedness decreases.
Also, a person’s estimate of any disaster is influenced by the worst recorded
instance, and most people do not expect an even worse thing to happen.
Therefore,
‘thinking fast and slow’ affects the manner we forecast any situation.
5- Our Inability To Understand Statistics Affects Thinking Fast And Slow
As we
have previously seen, our interpretation of data can often be biased. When
given a particular population consisting of different types of people and asked
to estimate the probability of any random person being of a particular type, we
are likely to base our estimate on the proportion of each type in the whole
population. The proportion of a particular class in a population is called the
base rate of that class. However, how much an instance conforms to the
stereotype of a particular class is called the representativeness of that
instance.
Often
in our estimates, we ignore the base rate and are guided by the
representativeness factor. This results in inaccurate estimates because rather
than being guided by data we are guided by our stereotypes. A tool to integrate
base rate and representativeness is to use Bayes’ formula, developed by
Reverend Thomas Bayes.
Base
rates are of two types, statistical and causal. Statistical base rates are
facts about a population to which a case belongs, but they are not relevant to
the individual case. Causal base rates, on the other hand, change our view of
how the individual case came to be. Although from a mathematical point of view
there is no difference between them, we are more likely to ignore statistical
base rate and give importance to the causal base rate. This is an instance of
bias.
Another
interesting statistical phenomenon is called Regression to the mean. This can
be seen while measuring the performance of a person (or any team or group) in
any field. Usually, a better than average performance is followed by a worse
performance, whereas a worse than average performance is followed by a better
one. This is because a better or worse than average performance is caused by a
stroke of good or bad luck, whereas the average determines the talent.
6- Our Memory Influences Our Behavior And Decisions
The
utility can be of two kinds, “experienced utility” and “decision utility”.
While experienced utility refers to the utility derived from hedonic
experiences, decision utility can be seen as wanting to enjoy something. If
people were fully rational, then these two utilities will be the same, as
people will make decisions to derive the utility they wanted. However, there
are discrepancies between the two.
Firstly,
let us see the two different theories of experiencing pain and pleasure. While
one theory suggests that the amount of pain and pleasure is influenced by the
peak value, the second theory suggests that it is influenced by the amount of
pain (or pleasure) at the end of the experience. Thus, the only important
factors are peak value and closing value (this is called the peak-end rule),
the duration of the experience is neglected.
In
fact, our decisions are influenced not by how we experience pleasure or pain
but by how our mind remembers them. What we learn from the past is to maximize the
qualities of our future memories, not necessarily our future experiences.
Peak-end
rule and duration neglect can be observed very well in stories. Mostly, the
focus in stories is usually on the critical events as well as the ending. The
length of the story hardly matters. Consider, for example, going on a vacation.
Usually, it is the most memorable moment that stays on with us. That is why
most vacation photographs are of important events, to help us recreate a story
of that vacation later.
7- Our Understanding Faces Illusions By A Narrative With Convincing & Consistent Context
Often a
good and well-constructed story might appear to us as true, even though it is
not. This is known as the narrative fallacy. In fact, a compelling narrative
may even foster the notion of inevitability, highlighting the role of skill and
conscious choices and diminishing the role of luck. It makes us assess the
quality of a decision not by whether the process was sound but by whether its
outcome was good or bad. Thus, if we see the story of any successful firm and
the choices they made, it would appear as if every time the same choices were
made, it would have resulted in success, even though this is not true, and an
element of luck is always involved.
There
is another illusion, the illusion of validity. Our System One loves to jump to
conclusions, even though the facts and evidence might point to otherwise. All it
takes is for a story to be coherent and be told in a confident tone. For
example, we tend to attribute individual differences in achievement to differences
in skill. This is a long-held belief among us. However, it is not based on much
evidence. In fact, luck has as much a role to play in success as skill and
talent if not more. Facts that do challenge basic assumptions, and thereby
threaten our livelihood and self-esteem, are simply not absorbed by our minds.
8- Inability Of Making Rational Decisions Even When People Think They Can
Most
economists assume that the average economic agent (i.e., a consumer or a
producer) is a rational being with selfish intent and consistent thoughts.
However, psychologists have proved that this is not actually true; people are
neither entirely rational nor completely selfish, and that their tastes are not
entirely stable.
It was
previously held that the actions and decisions of people are governed by what
is called the Utility Theory. Think rationally and choose the path which
results in the best result for you. However, the utility theory takes a beating
in many situations from the Prospect Theory.
Imagine
a couple of scenarios:
Scenario
1: You are given one thousand dollars and are asked to then
choose between getting a certain shot bonus of five hundred dollars, or
gambling with a fifty percent chance of winning another thousand dollars.
Scenario
2: You are given two thousand dollars and are then asked to
choose between a guaranteed loss of five hundred dollars and gambling with a
fifty percent chance of losing one thousand dollars.
If we
make a rational choice, our choice in both scenarios will be the same. However,
research shows that in the first scenario, people choose the guaranteed
pay-out, while in the second, they make a gamble!
The
truth is that we are afraid of losses more than we like to win. The perceived
losses are benchmarked to reference points which can be different for different
people. The starting position in the two scenarios with a different quantum of
money, makes the reference points different for both scenarios.
Conclusion Of Thinking Fast And Slow
If we
make a rational choice, our choice in both scenarios will be the same. However,
research shows that in the first scenario, people choose the guaranteed
pay-out, while in the second, they make a gamble!
The
truth is that we are afraid of losses more than we like to win. The perceived
losses are benchmarked to reference points which can be different for different
people. The starting position in the two scenarios with a different quantum of
money, makes the reference points different for both scenarios.
Our
minds can be seen as consisting of two separate but related systems of
thinking, system one and two. In other words, thinking fast and slow, while
the former is fast, automatic but quite inaccurate, the latter is slow,
voluntary but accurate. Often, the workings of system one, create biases and
illusions in our mind, since we tend to jump to conclusions instead of
carefully analyzing all facts. This is why, if we are to quickly form theories
after being presented with facts, the theories are most likely to be skewed and
biased.
The two
separate systems also affect how we experience feelings such as pleasure and
pain. We are generally risk-averse since the pain of losing is more than the
pleasure of gaining. This cannot be explained rationally, but rather is a
working of our feelings or emotions. Also, the pain or pleasure which we
remember out of an experience is most likely to be influenced by the peak and
ending values of the pain (or pleasure).
Comments
Post a Comment